Phonemic Awareness: What Does it Mean?
by Dr.
Kerry Hempenstall
http://www.educationoasis.com/resources/Articles/phonemic_awareness.htm
http://www.educationoasis.com/resources/Articles/phonemic_awareness.htm
translation in french coming soon...
RMIT University,
Melbourne, Australia Over the past two decades, but particularly in the last 10
years, there has been a burgeoning consensus about the critical importance of
phonemic awareness to beginning reading success, and about its role in specific
reading disability or dyslexia (Hatcher, Hulme, & Ellis, 1994; National
Reading Panel, 2000; Share, 1995; Stanovich, 1986). Various terms have been
employed as synonyms, such as phonological awareness, acoustic awareness,
phonetic awareness, auditory analysis, sound categorisation, phonemic
segmentation, phonological sensitivity, and phonemic analysis. Some authors
such as Goswami and Bryant (1990) reserve the term phonemic awareness to imply
awareness of individual phonemes; whereas, phonological awareness is a more
global term that includes the earlier stages, such as rhyme and syllable
awareness.
There
has been much discussion about how best to define phonemic awareness. Ball and
Blachman (1991) refer to the ability to recognise that a spoken word consists
of a sequence of individual sounds. Stanovich (1986) initially defined it as
the "conscious access to the phonemic level of the speech stream and some
ability to cognitively manipulate representations at this level" (p. 362).
Later, he suggested (1992, 1993) that the terms "conscious" and
"awareness" themselves have no acceptable definitions, and he
subsequently recommended phonological sensitivity as a generic term to
encompass a continuum from shallow to deep sensitivity. This term acknowledges
the wide range of tasks used to assess levels of sensitivity. Read (1991) too
was concerned about the term awareness, but because it implies a
dichotomy rather than a continuum. He preferred the expression access to
phonological structure. As these alternatives have not gained currency,
phonemic awareness will continue to be used here, acknowledging that the
definition has limitations.
What
is clear is that phonemic awareness concerns the structure of words rather than
their meaning. To understand the construction of our written code, readers need
to be able to reflect upon the spelling-to-sound correspondences. To understand
that the written word is composed of graphemes that correspond to phonemes (the
alphabetic principle), beginning readers must first have some understanding
that words are composed of sounds (phonemic awareness) rather than their
conceiving of each word as a single indivisible sound stream. This awareness
appears not to be a discrete state, but rather a sequence of development
ranging from simple to complex, or as Stanovich (1992, 1993b) would prefer -
from shallow to deep.
Although
some authors suggest slight variations in the sequence (Ehri et al., 2001), the
stages of phonological development toward deep phonemic awareness can be
delineated as below.
Recognition
that sentences are made up of words.
Recognition
that words can rhyme - then production thereof
Recognition
that words can be broken down into syllables - then production thereof
Recognition
that words can be broken down into onsets and rimes - then production thereof
Recognition
that words can begin with the same sound - then production of such words
Recognition
that words can end with the same sound -then production of such words
Recognition
that words can have the same medial sound(s) -then production of such words
Recognition
that words can be broken down into individual phonemes - then production
thereof
Recognition
that sounds can be deleted from words to make new words - then production
thereof
Ability
to blend sounds to make words
Ability
to segment words into constituent sounds
Phonemic
awareness is more complex than auditory discrimination, which is the ability to
perceive, for example, that cat and mat are different speech
productions, or words. To be able to describe how they are similar but
different, however, implies some level of phonemic awareness. Auditory
discrimination entails hearing a difference; whereas, phonemic awareness
entails a level of analysis of the constituent sounds. Young children are not
normally called upon to consider words at a level other than their meaning,
although experience with rhymes may be the first indication for children that
they can play with the structure of words.
Prior
to these finer intra-word discriminations, children need to appreciate that
spoken sentences (a rather continuous stream of sound without clear pauses) are
separable into discrete words (Liberman & Liberman, 1990). Adams (1990) and
Blachman (1984) warn that word consciousness (the awareness that spoken
language is composed of words) should not be assumed even in children with
several years schooling, although they report evidence that it may be taught
easily enough, even at a pre-school level. That school age children can lack
such fundamental knowledge may be difficult for adults to accept, but it
highlights the need in education to assume little, and assess pre-requisite
skills carefully. Their warning also challenges the view, held by some Whole
Language advocates (Goodman, 1979, 1986; Smith, 1975, 1992), that speaking and
reading involve equivalent "natural" processes for all children. The
implications of the Whole Language view are that the same environmental
conditions that occur during the development of speech are those best provided
for children learning to read. Liberman and Liberman (1990) among others (Gough
& Hillinger, 1980; Hirsch, 2001; Liberman, 1997) have provided a forceful
rebuttal of this equivalence perspective.
Having
discovered that sentences are composed of words, the next logical unit of
analysis is intra-word, at the syllable level. However, syllables can be
represented by any number of letters from one to eight. The word understand
has three syllables, each of a different number of letters. Un has
two, der has three, and stand has five letters. This
variability makes the syllable unit of limited value in analysing the reading
task (Bradley, 1990), and the catch is that one needs to have awareness at the
level of the phoneme in order to determine where best to decide the syllable
junctions. So, syllable awareness may have limited value as an early curriculum
focus.
Rhyme and Alliteration
The
recognition of rhyme may be the entry point to phonemic awareness development
for many children (Bryant, 1990). To be aware that words can have a similar
end-sound implies a critical step in metalinguistic understanding - that of
ignoring the meaning of a word in order to attend to its internal structure.
This leads to a new classification system, one in which words can be classified
according to end-sound rather than meaning. Bryant (1990) points to the
considerable amount of evidence indicating that children as young as three or
four years can make judgments such as when words rhyme, and when they begin
with the same sound (alliteration). He argues that sensitivity to rhyme makes
both a direct and indirect contribution to reading. Directly, it helps students
appreciate that words that share common sounds usually also share common letter
sequences. The child's subsequent sensitivity to common letter sequences then
makes a significant contribution to reading strategy development. Indirectly,
the recognition of rhyme promotes the refining of word analysis from larger
intra-word segments (such as rhyme) to analysis at the level of the phoneme
(the critical requirement for reading).
Studies
by Bryant, Bradley, McLean, and Crossland (1989) showed a very strong
relationship between rhyming ability at age three years and performance at
reading and spelling three years later. A number of such studies have
reinforced the value of such early exposure to rhyming games (e.g., Kirtley,
Bryant, Maclean, & Bradley, 1989). That rhyming and phoneme awareness are
related (through their common characteristic of requiring listening for sound
similarities and differences) was supported by an interesting finding of a
study by Lamb and Gregory (1993). They showed that children who were capable of
good discrimination of musical pitch also scored highly on tests of phonemic
awareness. Since pitch change is an important source of information in the
speech signal (Liberman, Cooper, Shankweiler, & Studdert-Kennedy, 1967), it
may be that sensitivity to small frequency changes, such as is involved in
phoneme recognition, is an important aspect of successful initial reading. Lamb
and Gregory (1993) raise the interesting possibility that musical training may
represent one of those pre-reading, home-based experiences that contribute to
the marked individual differences in phonemic awareness with which children
commence school.
Just
how valuable may be an instructional emphasis on rhyme has been questioned in
several studies (Wood, 2000). Whereas Bryant (1990) asserted that rhyme makes a
direct contribution to reading, others see rhyme subsumed under phonemic
awareness, so that rhyme awareness is only a phonological step on the way
towards phoneme awareness - a later state that certainly does influence reading
development. Perhaps the ultimate role for rhyme will be as a predictor of
progress towards reading success, as it has been shown to be a strong early
predictor of reading ability in longitudinal studies (Bradley & Bryant,
1983; Bryant et al., 1989). This is not to suggest that rhyming activities are
to be avoided, as they are enjoyable literacy activities. Engaging in rhyming
activities with stories may also have strong motivational influences on
children's attitudes to books and reading.
Onsets & Rimes
Treiman
(1991) has suggested a further stage in the development of phoneme awareness -
the intra-syllabic units of onset and rime. The onset of a syllable is its
initial consonant(s), and the rime is its vowel and any subsequent consonants
in the syllable. Thus, in the syllables sip-slip , the onsets are s
and sl , and the common rime is ip . Treiman's research
has argued for a stage between syllable awareness and phoneme awareness in
which children are much more sensitive to the onset-rime distinction than the
phoneme distinction. It has been asserted that this research holds promise for
programs of educational intervention in reading disability because of the greater
regularity of onset-rimes over individual letters (Felton, 1993). Thus, rime
phonograms such as ing, ight, ain have much more regularity than the
letters that form them. Knowing that strain and drain rhyme,
may allow for reading main and brain by analogy.
This
apparently generative strategy has led some researchers (Bowey, Cain, &
Ryan, 1992; Hulme & Snowling, 1992) to suggest that an emphasis on
onset-rime may be an especially valuable approach to teaching students with
dyslexia, as they tend to have relatively weak phonological skills. Further,
Bowey and Francis (1991) consider onset and rime the most effective focus for
phonological activities intended to promote beginning reading and spelling for
all children. They note that since most onsets in English are single
consonants, an early emphasis on the intra-syllabic onset/rime distinction in
the study of word structure is likely to hasten the development of awareness at
the more difficult phoneme level. Treiman (1991) has argued that the onset/rime
division is a natural one. Bradley (1990) agrees, and considers that it is
because rhymes correspond to rimes that most children develop such facility
with them at a relatively early age. The awareness of these larger sublexical
skills are viewed by Bruck (1992), Goswami and Bryant (1990), and by Tunmer and
Hoover (1993) as prerequisites to initial reading acquisition, their difficulty
level lying between that of syllable awareness and phoneme awareness (Bowey et
al., 1992; Bowey & Francis, 1991; Bruck & Treiman, 1990; Kirtley et
al., 1989). Spector (1995) perceives onset/rime as a potentially useful stage
in the development of oral segmentation skills. She recommends the strategy of
breaking such words into onset/rime as an intermediate step towards phonemic
segmentation for children who have difficulty in segmenting complex syllables.
Thus,
there may be a typical developmental sequence of phonological awareness. It
begins with awareness of words as a unit of analysis; then proceeds to the
awareness that words can share certain ending properties that we call rhyme, to
an awareness that words can be decomposed into syllables, then (possibly though
not definitely) more finely into sub-syllabic units called onsets and rimes, to
beginning, final, and medial properties, and then (and most importantly for
reading) into awareness of individual phonemes, the smallest unit of sound
analysis. A further developmental sequence involves the movement from a
recognition of such properties to a capacity to produce examples of them. Thus,
at one level one can nominate which pairs of words rhyme when presented orally;
at a higher level one can produce examples. It should be noted that the
description of the process as developmental does not imply spontaneous
development - for many students it needs to be taught (Lindamood, 1994).
The
issue of putting ages to stages is problematic partly because of the great
variation in the experience of children. Some children play with word structure
for several years before school, some have had no experience. The degree of
emphasis placed on phonemic awareness in preschool and school adds additional
variation, whilst the quality and explicitness of the instruction also make
significant contributions (National Reading Panel, 2000. There appears also to
be genetic predisposition toward ease or difficulty of acquisition among
children (Olson, Wise, Conners, Rack, & Fulker, 1989; Rack, Hulme, &
Snowling, 1993).
Thus,
these stages may be better considered as markers on the road to skilled reading,
rather than as a natural developmental sequence, and as susceptible to
environmental manipulation, such as early experiences and instruction.
Similarly, the rate with which students progress through the stages may vary,
and some stages may even appear to be skipped.
If
the stages represent a typical sequence, then approaches to teaching might
benefit from taking it into account. There may be some theoretical
justification for an interest in onset-rime, but it requires support from
intervention research before becoming a suitable component of the curriculum.
So, is an emphasis on teaching students to recognise onset-rime distinctions
(rather than at the phoneme level) more productive in initial (and, perhaps,
remedial) reading instruction than is teaching directly at the phoneme level. A
computer program developed by Wise, Olson and Treiman (1990) focussed on
onset-rimes in teaching beginning reading skills to normally-developing readers
and children with dyslexia. In this and the Olson and Wise (1992) studies, the
authors noted an advantage for the children taught in this manner over an
approach that segmented words after the vowel. The effect however was
ephemeral, and least pronounced in the more disabled students. Ehri and Robbins
(1992) findings were similar in that the poorer readers did not use
sub-syllabic units larger than the grapheme. This led them to suggest that the
onset-rime distinction is really the province of the more skilled reader, and
hence not a candidate for instruction prior to that at the phoneme level.
Goswami's
research (Goswami & Bryant, 1990) had suggested that, for young children,
words that share rimes are more readily decoded by analogy than are words that
share onsets or vowels. Bruck and Treiman (1992) provided some support for that
view, but as in the Wise et al. study, the measured advantage was lost within a
day. In fact, a day later the rime group demonstrated poorer performance than
the group taught onsets, and poorer than the group for which vowel analogy was
emphasised.
A
number of researchers now have questioned whether an onset-rime emphasis has
any useful role to play in beginning reading instruction. Nation and Hulme
(1997) express concern that such tasks are not predictive of reading and
spelling success. McMillan (2002) argues that it is alphabet knowledge rather
than rhyming ability that underpins any causal link to reading ability.
Further, Nation, Allen, and Hulme (2001) have questioned the benefit of
emphasising analogy as a worthwhile early strategy for reading unfamiliar
words. The intent of analogy reading is to allow children to decode an
unfamiliar printed word by observing that its spelling is similar to that of a
known word. In their study, however, children were not able to see such
orthographic similarities at all, leading to a conclusion that the analogy
technique is only able to be employed by those readers who already have
attained more advanced phonemic awareness (Wood, 2000).
Thus,
the results of research suggest caution regarding calls for introducing an
initial emphasis on onset-rime distinctions for beginning readers. It would be
judicious to ensure that beginners (and disabled readers) have, or develop, a
grounding in grapheme-phoneme relationships, either before (or simultaneous
with), such onset-rime emphasis (Munro, 1995). It is still unclear whether the
generally accepted developmental sequence necessarily provides the optimum
guidance for instruction. This question should be answered empirically, and a
number of researchers have attempted more fine-grained analysis to assist in
providing clearer instructional direction. Wise and Olson (1995) reported a
study indicating that adequate phonemic awareness skill was necessary if
children were to benefit from onset-rime instruction. When readers with
dyslexia were provided with phonemic awareness training through Auditory
Discrimination in Depth (Lindamood & Lindamood, 1969) simultaneously with
onset-rime computer-based training, reading results were markedly improved by
this addition of instruction at the level of the phoneme. The ADD program
emphasises phonemic awareness through a variety of oral/aural tasks, and by
teaching students awareness of kinaesthetic cues (mouth, tongue, lip position,
breath usage).
Nation
and Hulme (1997) and Hulme et al. (2002) also argue that it is likely to be
more profitable to emphasise phoneme awareness even from the beginning reading
stages. As is often the case, when several options are available and the
evidence is not adequate to clearly support one or the other, the emphasis is
most judiciously placed on the alternative that is most closely related to the
reading process. Thus, studies up to this stage have raised more questions than
answers about the instructional usefulness of onset-rime as a means of gently approaching
the difficult phoneme concept.
Phoneme Awareness
Awareness
at the level of the phoneme has particular significance for the acquisition of
reading because of its role in the development of the alphabetic principle -
that the written word is simply a means of codifying the sound properties of
the spoken word. In order to decode the written word, the child needs to
appreciate the logic of the writing system and, as a prerequisite, the logic of
oral word production.
There
are two requirements of beginning reading for which phonemic awareness becomes
immediately relevant: phonemic analysis (segmentation) and phonemic synthesis
(blending). For most children, the ability to produce the finer discrimination
of phonemes begins in about Year I of their schooling (Ball, 1993). Individual
phonemes are more difficult to specify because their acoustic values vary with
the phonemes that precede and follow them in a word (a phenomenon called
co-articulation); whereas, syllables have relatively constant values in a word
and hence should be more readily recognised. The fact that consonants are
"folded" into vowels can be understood by noting the different tongue
positions for the beginning /d/ sound when it is followed by /oo/ and by /i/.
In
most children the ability to synthesise (blend) sounds into words occurs
earlier than analytic (segmentation) skills (Bryen & Gerber, 1987;
Caravolas & Bruck, 1993; Solomons, 1992; Torgesen et al., 1992; Yopp,
1992). Thus, it is easier to respond with the word cat when presented
with the sounds c - at or c-a-t , than it is to supply c-a-t
when asked to tell what sounds you hear in cat .
Tasks
used to assess beginning (or shallow) phonemic awareness tend to emphasise
sensitivity to rhyme and alliteration; for example, finding a word that begins
or ends with the same sound as the stimulus word. A more complex task would
involve the manipulation, or separation of sounds in a word, for example, What
is the first sound you hear in cat ? What word is left if you remove
the /t/ from "stand"? (Torgesen et al., 1994). Other tasks used for
assessment may include counting the sounds in words, adding, deleting or
manipulating sounds, and categorising sounds at the beginning, middle, or end
of words.
There
are now numerous normed and unnnormed tests available. Some are available from
publishers, such as the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP)
(Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999) whilst some are free from the Net,
such as Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills ( DIBELS) (University of Oregon, 2002a) or the Abecedarian
Reading Assessment ( Wren & Watts, 2002). A useful resource in making
decisions about which test to employ is an extensive and thorough review by
Kame'enui (2002).
As
indicated above, deeper levels of awareness (i.e., at the phoneme level) tend
to develop during first grade upon exposure to reading instruction. Some have
argued then that phonemic awareness may be a consequence of learning to read
rather than a causal factor in its development (Morais et al., 1987; Morais,
1991). There is increasing consensus that the data are best explained by
considering the relationship between phonemic awareness and reading development
as a reciprocal one (Stanovich, 1992). A threshold phonemic awareness level may
be necessary (though not sufficient) for beginning reading development, but as
reading develops - increasingly the student becomes more sensitive and better
able to manipulate sounds at the phoneme level.
The
acquisition of phonemic awareness is not guaranteed simply through maturation;
in fact, about a third of students require varying degrees of assistance to
promote its development (Adams, 1990). So,
what do you teach? Do you purchase one of the many available texts such as the
classroom curriculum by Adams, Foorman, Lundberg, and Beeler (1998), and work
your way through the oral activities or do you contrive your own? Is the
developmental sequence important? Should you gently guide students through the
sequence, using only activities related to that level, or can you provide
students with a richer range of activities at any one time? Should you focus
directly on phoneme awareness (rather than on less sophisticated phonological
processes like rhymes) from the beginning (Foorman et al., 2003)? Should you include
letters (graphemes) in your otherwise oral curriculum (Byrne &
Fielding-Barnsley, 1993; National Reading Panel, 2000)? If so, when? Perhaps
you should bypass the oral phonemic awareness activities, and move directly to
the phonic processes of segmenting and blending (using letters) because they
are activities more directly salient to reading? Maybe phonemic awareness is
best considered as a consequence of reading development? And what about the
student who is resistant to the activity-based curriculum, perhaps with other
phonological problems such as slow naming speed ( Al Otaiba & Fuchs, 2002)?
Do you introduce more formal direct instruction procedures over an extended
period of time (Lyon, 2001; Wright & Jacobs, 2003)? There are many questions
not entirely resolved, but for a detailed and helpful resource the University of Oregon (2002b) site Big Ideas in
Beginning Reading should be examined.
The
National Reading Panel Report (2000) indicated that large effect sizes were
possible when instruction was directed systematically and explicitly at one or
two types of phonemic awareness activities provided to small groups, and
involved associating phonemes with letters (such as segmenting and blending).
As to who might require more intensive and extended assistance, Torgesen (1998)
recommends an identification procedure involving administration of a test of
knowledge of letter names or sounds and a measure of phonemic awareness.
Students who do not do well on these tests are likely to struggle with reading
unless additional support is provided. The Panels view was that this focus was
so important that all students should have the opportunity to benefit from
phonemic awareness activities in their first year of school. Those studies that
provided activities for less than a half hour per day to a total of about 20
hours were effective and efficient.
The
issue of when to introduce phonemic awareness activities/instruction has also
been investigated. Byrne, Fielding-Barnsley, and Ashley (2000) report that it
is not only the attainment of phonemic awareness that is important in learning
to read, but also its speed of acquisition. In a longitudinal study, they noted
that poor readers in grade 5 were those who, though they eventually achieved
reasonable levels of phonemic awareness, were slow to grasp it. Perhaps there
is a window of opportunity when phonological processes can become the driving
force behind initial reading development. If reading development is not
phonologically informed then students may adopt less viable strategies, such as
guessing and memorisation of shapes. If that occurs, phonemic awareness may
subsequently develop, but will not necessarily be employed by the student whose
strategies have become entrenched. Perhaps this is the reason why it can take
four times as much intervention to improve a child's reading skills if help is
delayed until grade 4 than if it is begun in the first year of school (Hall
& Moats, 1999).
The
role of fluency in promoting reading comprehension was brought to the attention
of many because of its status in the report of the National Reading Panel
(2000). Less well known is an increasing interest in promoting fluency across a
range of curriculum areas ( Binder , Haughton, & Bateman, 2002; Lindsley,
1996). Binder et al. suggest that while mastery is important, real expertise in
phonemic awareness skills is not present until students can effortlessly and
quickly perform the tasks. Thus, they suggest students should aim to be able to
blend sounds to form words at a minimum of 10 per minute, segment words into
sounds by moving colored blocks to indicate the sounds at a rate of at least 40
per minute, and construct new words through substituting one phoneme for
another at a minimum rate of 15 per minute. This suggestion certainly offers
another dimension for teachers wishing to ensure all their students develop a
strong phonological basis for literacy.
Of
course, a classroom emphasis on phonological processes assumes that teachers
already have the necessary deep understanding of phonemic awareness required to
teach it effectively. This assumption may not be warranted, as research has
indicated that many teachers do not themselves have a solid foundation in their
own phonemic awareness, and few have received the level of training that
produces the supra-skill level important in awakening children's fine-grained
sensitivity to the sound structure of words (Lindamood, 1994; Mather, Bos,
& Babur, 2001; Moats, 1994). For example, in one study (Mather et al.) only
2% of teachers-in-training and 19% of working teachers knew that the word box
is constructed from four speech sounds. It is not easy for adults to
ignore entrenched spelling patterns when confronted with phonemic tasks (Labov,
2003). Students whose teacher themselves have phonological deficiencies display
lower levels of reading skills as a consequence (Lindamood, 1994). In many
teacher-training facilities, pre-service instruction in these areas is not
among the priorities in developing a teacher education curriculum on literacy.
Hence, many teachers are likely to need retraining if the results of phonemic
awareness research into beginning reading are to be put into practice
successfully.
REFERENCES
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning
to read: Thinking and learning about print . Cambridge, MA
MIT Press. Adams, M. J., Foorman, B. R., Lundberg, I., & Beeler, T. (1998). Phonemic awareness in young children : A classroom curriculum . Baltimore, MA: Brookes Publishing.
Al Otaiba, S. & Fuchs, D. (2002). Characteristics of children who are unresponsive to early literacy intervention: A review of the literature. Remedial and Special Education, 23, 300-316.
Ball, E. W. (1993). Phonological awareness: What's important and to whom? Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5 , 141-159.
Ball, E. W., & Blachman, B. A. (1991). Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling. Reading Research Quarterly, 25 , 49-66.
Binder , C., Haughton, E., & Bateman, B. (2002). Fluency: Achieving true mastery in the learning process. Retrieved 1/3/2003 from http://curry.edschool.virginia.edu/sped/projects/ose/papers/
Blachman, B. A. (1984). Language analysis skills and early reading acquisition. In G. P. Wallach, & K. G. Butler (Eds.), Language learning disabilities in school age children (pp. 271-287). Baltimore Williams & Wilkins.
Bowey, J. A., & Francis, J. (1991). Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12 , 91-121.
Bowey, J. A., Cain, M. T., & Ryan, S. M. (1992). A reading-level design study of phonological skills underlying fourth-grade children's word reading difficulties. Child Development, 63 , 999-1011.
Bradley, L. (1990). Rhyming connections in learning to read and spell. In P. D. Pumphrey, & C. D. Elliot (Eds.), Children's difficulties in reading, spelling and writing: Challenges and responses (pp. 83-100). London The Falmer Press.
Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50 , 156-178.
Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological development and reading. In P. D. Pumfrey, & C. D. Elliot (Eds.), Children's difficulties in reading, spelling and writing: Challenges and responses (pp. 63-82). London The Falmer Press.
Bryant, P. E., Bradley, L., Maclean, M., & Crossland, J. (1989). Nursery rhymes, phonological skills and reading. Journal of Child Language, 16 , 407-428
Bryen, D. N., & Gerber, A. (1987). Metalinguistic abilities and reading: A focus on phonological awareness. Reading, Writing & Learning Disabilities, 3 , 357-367.
Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A one-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85 , 104-111.
Byrne, B., Fielding-Barnsley, R, & Ashley, L. (2000). Effects of preschool phoneme identity training after six years: Outcome level distinguished from rate of response. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92 , 659-667.
Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children's phonological awareness: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1-30.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel's meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36 , 250-287.
Felton, R. H. (1993). Effects of instruction on the decoding skills of children with phonological-processing problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26 (4), 583-589.
Foorman, B. R., Chen, D-T., Carlson, C., Moats, L., Francis, D. J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2003). The necessity of the alphabetic principle to phonemic awareness instruction. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16 , 289-324.
Gough, P. B., & Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30 , 179-196.
Hall, S. H., & Moats, L. C. (1999 ). Straight talk about reading: How parents can make a difference during the early years . Chicago: Contemporary Books.
Hatcher, P., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. (1994). Ameliorating reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65 , 41-57.
Hirsch E. D. (2001, Spring). Romancing the child. Education Matters, 34-39. Retrieved 11/3/2003 from www.educationnext.org/2001sp/34.html
Hulme, C., Hatcher, P. J., Nation, K., Brown, A., Adams, J., & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82 , 2-28.
Kame'enui, E. J. (2002). An analysis of reading assessment instruments for K-3 . University of Oregon, OR: Institute for the Development of Educational Achievement. Retrieved 6/6/2003 from http://idea.uoregon.edu/assessment/final_report.pdf .
Kirtley, C., Bryant, P. E., Maclean, M. J., & Bradley, L. L. (1989). Rhyme, rime and the onset of reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48 , 224-245.
Labov, L. (2003). When ordinary children fail to read. Reading Research Quarterly, 38 , 128-131.
Lamb, S. J., & Gregory, A. H. (1993). The relationship between music and reading in beginning readers. Educational Psychology, 13, 19-27.
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431-461.
Liberman, A.M. (1997). How theories of speech affect research in reading and writing. In B. A. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia . Mahway, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.
Liberman, I. Y., & Liberman, A. M. (1990). Whole language vs code emphasis: Underlying assumptions and their implications for reading instruction. Annals of Dyslexia, 40 , 51-75.
Lindamood, C., & Lindamood, P. C. (1969). Auditory Discrimination in Depth. USA Riverside Publishers.
Lindamood, P. C. (1994). Issues in researching the link between phonological awareness, learning disabilities, and spelling. In G. R. Lyon, (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 351-374). Baltimore, PH: Brookes Publishing Co.
Lindsley, O. R. (1996). Is fluency free-operant response-response chaining? The Behavior Analyst, 19 (2), 211-224.
Lyon, G. R. (2001). Measuring success: Using assessments and accountability to raise student achievement . Subcommittee on Education Reform Committee on Education and the Workforce U.S. House of Representatives Washington, D.C. Retrieved 6/6/2003 from http://www.nrrf.org/lyon_statement3-01.htm
Macmillan, B. M. (2002). Rhyme and reading: A critical review of the research methodology. Journal of Research in Reading, 25 (1), 4-42.
Mather, N., Bos, C., & Babur, N. (2001). Perceptions and knowledge of preservice and inservice teachers about early literacy instruction. Journal of Learning Disabilities , 34 , 472-482.
Moats, L. C. (1994). The missing foundation in teacher education: Knowledge of the structure of spoken and written language. Annals of Dyslexia, 44 , 81-102.
Morais, J. (1991). Phonological awareness: A bridge between language & literacy. In D. Sawyer, & B. Fox (Eds.), Phonological awareness in reading: The evolution of current perspectives . (pp. 31 - 71). New York Springer-Verlag.
Morais, J., Alegria, J., & Content, A. (1987). The relationship between segmental analysis and alphabetic literacy: An interactive view. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7 , 1-24.
Munro, J. (1995). Explaining developmental dyslexia Orthographic processing difficulties. Australian Journal of Remedial Education, 27 (1), 5-15.
Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic segmentation, not onset-rime segmentation predicts early reading and spelling skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 32 , 154-167.
Nation, K., Allen, R., & Hulme, C. (2001). The limitations of orthographic analogy in early reading development: Performance on the clue-word task depends on phonological priming and elementary decoding skill, not the use of orthographic analogy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80 , 75-94.
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction . Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Retrieved 12/2/03 from http://www.nationalreadingpanel.org
Olson, R. K., & Wise, B. W. (1992). Reading on the computer with orthographic and speech feedback. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4 , 108-144.
Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J. P., & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22 , 339-348.
Rack, J. P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (1993). Learning to read: A theoretical synthesis. Advances in Child Development & Behaviour, 24 , 99-132.
Read, C. (1991). Access to syllable structure in language and learning. In S. A. Brady, & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 119-124). NJ Lawrence Erlbaum.
Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55 , 151-218.
Smith, F. (1975). Comprehension and learning: A conceptual framework for teachers . New York Richard C. Owen.
Smith, F. (1992). Learning to read: The never-ending debate. Phi Delta Kappan, 74 , 432-441.
Solomons, B. (1992). Phonemic awareness training: An early intervention program . NSW, Australia Macquarie University
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21 , 360-406.
Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculation on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307-342). Hillsdale, NJ Laurence Erlbaum.
Stanovich, K. E. (1993). The language code: Issues in word recognition. In S. R. Yussen, & M. C. Smith (Eds.), Reading across the life span (pp. 111-135). New York Springer-Verlag.
Torgesen, J. K., & Bryant, B. (1994a). Test of Phonological Awareness . Austin, TX Pro-Ed.
Torgesen, J. K. (1998). Catch them before they fall: Identification and assessment to prevent reading failure in young children. American Educator, Spring/Summer. Retrieved 12/2/03 from : http://www.ldonline.org/ld_indepth/reading/torgeson_catchthem.html
Torgesen, J. K., Morgan, S. T., & Davis, C. (1992). Effects of two types of phonological awareness training on word learning in kindergarten children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 364-370.
Treiman, R. (1991). The role of intrasyllabic units in learning to read and spell. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 65-106). Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum.
University of Oregon. (2002a). What are the Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills or DIBELS ? . Retrieved 5/10/2003 from http://reading.uoregon.edu/assessment/dibels_what.php
University of Oregon. (2002b). Big ideas in beginning reading. Retrieved 5/10/2003 from http://reading.uoregon.edu
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing . Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Wise, B. W., & Olson, R. K. (1995). Computer-based phonological awareness and reading instruction. Annals of Dyslexia, 45 , 99-122.
Wise, B. W., Olson, R. K., & Treiman, R. (1990). Subsyllabic units as aids in beginning readers word learning Onset-rime versus post-vowel segmentation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49 , 1-19.
Wood, C. (2000). Rhyme awareness, orthographic analogy use, phonemic awareness and reading: An examination of relationships Educational Psychology, 20 (1), 5-15.
Wren, S., & Watts, J. (2002). Abecedarian Reading Assessment . Southwest Educational Development Laboratory. Retrieved 12/2/03 from http://www.balancedreading.com/assessment/abecedarian.html
Wright, J., & Jacobs, B. (2003). Teaching phonological awareness and metacognitive strategies to children with reading difficulties: A comparison of the two instructional methods. Educational Psychology, 23 (1), 17-45.
Yopp, H. K. (1992). Developing phonemic awareness in young children. The Reading Teacher, 45 , 696-703.
Author Information:
Dr. Kerry Hempenstall
Department of Psychology and Intellectual Disability Studies,
Royal Melbourne Institute of Technology (RMIT),
Plenty Rd., Bundoora,
Victoria, Australia. 3083
Sciences cognitives et lecture
sur le blog ...
Took me time to read all the comments, but I really enjoyed the article.
RépondreSupprimerI blog often and I truly appreciate your content.
RépondreSupprimerI like the helpful information you provide in your articles.
RépondreSupprimerThe website opened and I think it's okay.
RépondreSupprimerI always think about what is. It seems to be a perfect article that seems to blow away such worries.
RépondreSupprimerVery wonderful info can be found on website.
RépondreSupprimerI reckon something truly special in this website.
RépondreSupprimerI am impressed with this internet site, very I am a big fan .
RépondreSupprimerAbsolutely indited subject matter, Really enjoyed reading through.
RépondreSupprimerTerrific article! That is the type of information that are meant to be shared across the net.
RépondreSupprimerI am continually browsing online for tips that can facilitate me. Thank you!
RépondreSupprimerI am constantly thought about this, appreciate it for putting up.
RépondreSupprimerVery good article. I certainly appreciate this website. Keep it up!
RépondreSupprimerWhat’s up to all, it’s genuinely a good for me to visit this website, it includes helpful Information.
RépondreSupprimerGreate pieces. Keep writing such kind of info on your site. Im really impressed by your blog.
RépondreSupprimerPretty! This has been an extremely wonderful post. Many thanks for providing this info.
RépondreSupprimerGreat delivery. Great arguments. Keep up the amazing effort.
RépondreSupprimerI like this blog its a master peace ! Glad I observed this on google.
RépondreSupprimerPlease stay us up to date like this. Thank you for sharing.
RépondreSupprimerI gotta favorite this site it seems extremely helpful very helpful
RépondreSupprimerIt absolutely helpful and it has aided me out loads.
RépondreSupprimerI am hoping to give a contribution & assist other users like its helped me. Great job.
RépondreSupprimerYou should take part in a contest for one of the best blogs on the web.
RépondreSupprimer
RépondreSupprimerI read your article very impressively I want to write something like this
RépondreSupprimerthanks for sharing great blog article thanks again
RépondreSupprimerVery good article. I’m facing many of these issues as well
RépondreSupprimerthanks for the good ideas
RépondreSupprimerFirst You got a great blog
RépondreSupprimerThis is the perfect
RépondreSupprimerThanks for the info I will try to figure it out for more
RépondreSupprimerI’m glad that you simply shared this useful info with us
RépondreSupprimerReally a great addition. I have read this marvelous post
RépondreSupprimerI am always searching for quality posts and articles
RépondreSupprimerYour blog got me to learn a lot, thanks for sharing, nice article
RépondreSupprimerThis article is truly amazing. Appreciative for sharing such mind blowing information.
RépondreSupprimerFirst You got a great blog