Chapter I
A LETTER TO JOHNNY'S MOTHER
Dear Mary:
I have decided
to start this book with a letter to you. You know that the idea came to me when
I offered to help John, with his reading. It's really his book — or yours. So
the only proper way to start it is with the words “Dear Mary.”
You remember when I began to work with Johnny half a year ago. That was
when he was twelve and they put him back into sixth grade because he was unable
to read and couldn't possibly keep up with the work in junior high. So I told
you that I knew of a way to teach reading that was altogether different from
what they do in schools or in remedial reading courses or anywhere else. Well,
you trusted me, and you know what has happened since. Today Johnny can read — not
perfectly, to be sure, but anyone can see that in a few more months he will
have caught up with other boys of his age. And he is happy again: You and I and
everyone else can see that he is a changed person.
I think Johnny
will go to college. He has a very good mind, as you know, and I don't see why
he shouldn't become a doctor or a lawyer or an engineer. There is a lot in
Johnny that has never come to the surface because of this reading trouble.
Since I started
to work with Johnny, I have looked into this whole reading business. I worked
my way through a mountain of books and articles on the subject, I talked to
dozens of people, and I spent many hours in classrooms, watching what was going
on.
What I found is
absolutely fantastic. The teaching of reading — all over the United States, in
all the schools, in all the textbooks — is totally wrong and flies in the face
of all logic and common sense. Johnny couldn't read until half a year ago for
the simple reason that nobody ever showed him how. Johnny's only problem was
that he was unfortunately exposed to an ordinary American school.
You know that I was born and raised in Austria. Do you know that there
are no remedial reading cases in Austrian schools? Do you know that there are
no remedial reading cases in Germany, in France, in Italy, in Norway, in Spain —
practically anywhere in the world except in the United States? Do you know that
there was no such thing as remedial reading in this country either until about
thirty years ago? Do you know that the teaching of reading never was a problem
anywhere in the world until the United States switched to the present method around about 1925?
This sounds incredible, but it is true. One of the articles on reading
that I found was by a Dr. Ralph C. Preston, of the University of Pennsylvania,
who reported on his experiences on a trip through Western Germany in the April, 1953, Elementary School Journal. Dr. Preston visited a number of
classrooms in Hamburg and Munich. “After the experience of hearing these German children read aloud,” he
says, “I began to attach some credence to a generally expressed opinion of German
teachers that before the end of Grade 2 almost
any child can read orally (without
regard to degree of comprehension) almost anything in print!”
Of course, Dr. Preston, being an American educator, didn't draw the
obvious conclusion from what he saw. The explanation is simply that the method
used over there works, and the method
used in our schools does not. We too could have perfect readers in all schools
at the end of second grade if we taught our children by the system used in
Germany.
Now, what is this system? It's very simple. Reading means getting
meaning from certain combinations of letters. Teach the child what each letter
stands for and he can read.
Ah no, you say, it can't be that simple. But it is. Let me give you an
illustration.
I don't know whether you know any shorthand. Let's suppose you don't.
Let's suppose you decide to learn how to read English shorthand.
Right away you say that nobody learns how to read shorthand. People who want to know shorthand learn how to write it; the reading of it comes by the
way.
Exactly. That's why shorthand is such a good illustration of this whole
thing. It’s just a system of getting words on paper. Ordinary writing is
another such system. Morse code is a
third. Braille is a fourth. And so it goes. There are all sorts of systems of translating spoken words into a series of symbols so that they can be written down and read back.
third. Braille is a fourth. And so it goes. There are all sorts of systems of translating spoken words into a series of symbols so that they can be written down and read back.
Now the way to
learn any such system is to learn to write and to read it at the same time. And
how do you do that? The obvious answer is, By tacking up one symbol after
another and learning how to write it and how to recognize it. Once you are
through the whole list of symbols, you can read and write; the rest is simply
practice — learning to do it more and more automatically.
Since the dawn
of time people have learned mechanical means of communication in this way — smoke
signals and drums in the jungle and flag language and I don't know what all.
You take up one item after another, learn what it stands for, learn how to
reproduce it and how to recognize it, and there you are.
Shorthand, as I said, is an excellent example. I don't know any English
shorthand myself, but I went to a library and looked up the most widely used
manual of the Gregg system, the Functional
Method by L. A. Leslie. Sure enough, it tells you about the symbols one
after the other, starting out with the loop that stands for the long a in ache,
make, and cake. After a few lessons,
you are supposed to know the shape of all the shorthand “letters,” and from there
on it's just a matter of practice and picking up speed.
Our system of writing — the alphabet — was invented by the Egyptians
and the Phoenicians somewhere around 1500 b.c. Before the invention of the
alphabet there was only picture writing — a picture of an ox meant “ox,” a
picture of a house meant “house,” and so on. (The Chinese to this day have a
system of writing with symbols that stand for whole words.) As soon as people
had an alphabet, the job of reading and writing was tremendously simplified.
Before that, you had to have a symbol for every word in the language - 10,000,
20,000 or whatever the vocabulary range was. Now, with the alphabet, all you
had to learn was the letters. Each letter stood for a certain sound, and that
was that. To write a word — any word — all you had to do was break it down into
its sounds and put the corresponding letters on paper.
So, ever since 1500 b.c. people all over the world — wherever an
alphabetic system of writing was used — learned how to read and write by the
simple process of memorizing the sound of each letter in the alphabet. When a schoolboy in
ancient Rome learned to read, he didn't learn that the written word mensa meant a table, that is, a certain
piece of furniture with a flat top and legs. Instead, he began by learning that
the letter m stands for the sound you
make when you put your lips together, that e
means the sound that comes out when you open your mouth about halfway, that
n is like m but with the lips open and the teeth together, that s has a hissing sound, and that a means the sound made by opening your
mouth wide. Therefore, when he saw the written word mensa for the first time, he could read it right off and learn,
with a feeling of happy discovery, that this collection of letters meant a
table. Not only that, he could also write the word down from dictation without
ever having seen it before. And not only that,
he could do this with practically every word in the language.
This is not
miraculous, it's the only natural system of learning how to read. As I said,
the ancient Egyptians learned that way, and the Greeks and the Romans, and the
French and the Germans, and the Dutch and the Portuguese, and the Turks and the
Bulgarians and the Estonians and the Icelanders and the Abyssinians — every
single nation throughout history that used an alphabetic system of writing.
Except, as I said before, twentieth-century Americans — and other
nations in so far as they have followed our example. And what do we use
instead? Why, the only other possible system of course — the system that was in
use before the invention of the alphabet in 1500 b.c. We have decided to forget that we write with letters and learn to
read English as if it were Chinese. One word after another after another after
another. If we want to read materials with a vocabulary of 10,000 words, then we
have to memorize 10,000 words; if we want to go to the 20,000 word range, we
have to learn, one by one, 20,000 words; and so on. We have thrown 3,500 years
of civilization out the window and have gone back to the Age of Hammurabi.
You don't
believe me? I assure you what I am saying is literally true. Go to your school
tomorrow morning — or if John has brought home one of his readers, look at it.
You will immediately see that all the words in it are learned by endless
repetition. Not a sign anywhere that letters correspond to sounds and that
words can be worked out by pronouncing the letters. No. The child is told what
each word means and then they are mechanically, brutally hammered into his brain.
Like this:
All the reading
books used in aIl our schools, up through fourth and fifth and sixth grade, are
collections of staff hke that. Our cledren learn the word sot by reading over and
over again abouta duck or a pig or a goat that sot and sot and sot. And so with
every word in the language.
“We will
look,” said Susan.
“Yes,
yes,” said all the children.
“We will
look and find it?”
So all the boys and
girls looked.
They looked and looked
for it.
But they did not find
it.
Or this:
“Quark, quark,” said the duck.
He wanted something.
He did not want to get
out.
He did not want to go
to the farm
He did not want to
eat.
He sat and sat and
sat.
All the reading
books used in all our schools, up through fourth and fifth and sixth grade, are
collections of stuff like that. Our children learn the word sat by reading over and over again about
a duck or a pig or a goat that sat and sat and sat. And so with every word in
the language.
Every word in the language! You know what that means? It means that if
you teach reading by this system, you can't use ordinary reading matter for
practice. Instead, all children for three, four, five, six years have to work
their way up through a battery of carefully designed readers, each one
containing all the words used in the previous one plus a strictly limited
number of new ones, used with the exactly “right” amount of repetition. Our
children don't read Andersen's Fairy
Tales any more or The Arabian Nights or
Mark Twain or Louisa May Alcott or the Mary Poppins books or the Dr. Doolittle
books or anything interesting and
worth white, because they can’t. It
so happens that the writers of these classic children's books wrote without
being aware of our Chinese system of teaching reading. So Little Women contains words like grieving and serene, and Tom Sawyer has ague and inwardly, and
Bulfinch's Age of Fable has nymph and deity and incantations. If
a child that has gone to any of our schools faces the word nymph for the first time, he is absolutely helpless because nobody
has ever told him how to sound out n and
y and m and ph and read the
word off the page.
So what does he
get instead? He gets those series of horrible, stupid, emasculated, pointless,
tasteless little readers, the stuff and guff about Dick and Jane or Alice and
Jerry visiting the farm and having birthday parties and seeing animals in the
zoo and going through dozens and dozens of totally unexciting middle-class,
middle-income, middle-I.Q. children's activities that offer opportunities for
reading "Look, look" or «Yes, yes" or «Come, come" or «See
the funny, funny animal.» During the past half year I read a good deal of this
material and I don't wish that experience on anyone.
Who writes these books? Let me explain this to you in detail, because
there is the nub of the whole problem.
There are one or
two dozen textbook houses in America. By far the more lucrative part of their
business is the publication of readers for elementary schools. There are
millions of dollars of profit in these little books. Naturally, the competition
is tremendous. So is the investment; so is the sales effort; so is the effort
that goes into writing, editing, and illustrating these books.
Now, with our
Chinese word-learning system you can't produce a series of readers by printing
nice, interesting collections of stuff children of a certain age might like to
read. Oh no. Every single story, every single sentence that goes into these
books has to be carefully prepared and carefully checked to make sure that each
word is one of the 637 that the poor child is supposed to have memorized up to
that point—or if it's the 638th word, that it appears in just the right context
for optimum guesswork and is then repeated seventeen times at carefully
worked-out intervals.
Naturally, the stupendous and frighteningly idiotic work of concocting
this stuff can only be done by tireless teamwork of many educational drudges.
But if the textbook house put only the drudges on
the title page, that wouldn't look impressive enough to beat the competition.
So there has to be a “senior author” — someone with a national reputation who
teaches how to teach reading at one of the major universities.
And that’s why
each and every one of the so-called authorities in this field is tied up with a
series of readers based on the Chinese word-learning method. As long as you
used that method, you have to buy some $30 worth per child of Dr. So-and-so's
readers; as soon as you switch to the common-sense method of teaching the sounds
of the letters, you can give them a little primer and then proceed immediately
to anything from the Reader's Digest
to Treasure Island.
I have
personally met some of the leading authorities in the field of reading. They
are all very nice ladies and gentlemen, and obviously sincere and well meaning.
But they are firmly committed to the
application of the word method, and it would be inhuman to expect from them an
objective point of view.
Consequently
it’s utterly impossible to find anyone inside the official family of the
educators saying anything even slightly favorable to the natural method of
teaching reading. Mention the alphabetic method or phonetics or “phonics” and
you immediately arouse derision, furious hostility, or icy silence.
For instance, in the May 1952 Catholic
Educator, Monsignor Clarence E.
Elwell published an article "Reading: The Alphabet and Phonics."
Monsignor Elwell is Superintendent of Schools of the Diocese of Cleveland and
knows what he is talking about. He says: "In a language based on an
alphabetic (that is, phonetic) method of coding the spoken word, the only
sensible way to teach how to decode the written symbols is (i) by teaching the phonetic code, that is, the
alphabet, and (2) the manner of coding —
letter by letter, left to right. It is as nonsensical to use a whole
word method for beginning reading as it would be to teach the Morse code on a
whole word basic.... A child who has been taught the code and how to use it … gains
a confident habit in attacking words. Instead of guessing when he comes to a
new word, as he did when taught by the sight word method, he now works through
a word and to the surprise of the teachers usually comes up with the right
answer.... After four years' experiment with the introduction of a strong
program of phonics at the very beginning of grade one, the experimenter finds
teachers convinced and children apparently happier in their success.”
What do you
think happened when Monsignor Elwell said publicly that our whole system of
teaching reading is nonsense? Absolutely nothing. So far as I know, none of the
reading "experts" has paid the slightest attention to the Cleveland
experiment.
Or take the
case of the late Dr. Leonard Bloomfield, professor of linguistics at Yale. Dr.
Bloomfield wasn't just any scholar in the field of language; he was universally
recognized as the greatest American linguist of modern times. His masterpiece was
a book simply called Language, published
in 1933.
In the last few
pages of that book, Bloomfield dealt with the teaching of English and reading
in our schools. "Our schools," he wrote, “are utterly benighted in
linguistic matters.... Nothing could be more discouraging than to read our
“educationalists” treatises on methods of teaching children to read. The size
of this book does not permit a discussion of their varieties of confusion on
this subject.”
Several years
later, Bloomfield took time out to prepare an alphabetic-phonetic primer, based
on strictly scientific principles. It was an excellent piece of work, carefully
designed to teach children quickly and painlessly. After Bloomfield's death in
1949 his literary executor offered the manuscript to every single elementary
textbook publisher in the United States. Not one of them considered it. As I am
writing, the book is still unpublished.
(The primer was
published in 1961 : Leonard Bloomfield (1887-1949),
Clarence Lewis Barnhart, Let’s Read, A Linguistic
Approach, Wayne State University Press, 1 juin 1961 - 465 pages).
The introduction to this Bloomfield primer was, however, published as
an article in the Elementary English
Review in April and May, 1942. I ran across that article eight or ten years
ago and that's what started me on this
whole business. Taking the ideas of that article and applying them in homemade
fashion, I taught my eldest daughter Anne to read when she was five years old.
Well, you know Anne: she's ten now and reads anything and everything, all the
time. Here is what Bloomfield told the country's elementary English teachers
twelve years ago: “The most serious drawback of all the English reading
instruction known to me ... is the drawback of the word-method.... The child
who fails to grasp the content of what he reads is usually a poor reader in the
mechanical sense.... If you want to play the piano with feeling and expression,
you must master the keyboard and learn to use your fingers on it. The chief
source of difficulty in getting the content of reading is imperfect mastery of
the mechanics of reading.... We must train the child to respond vocally to the
sight of letters....”
And what did
the teachers and reading experts do after the greatest scientist in the field
had explained to them their mistake? Absolutely nothing. Except that several
years later, in 1948, Dr. William S. Gray, of the University of Chicago,
published a book, On Their Own in
Reading. There, in the first
chapter, was a lengthy quotation from Bloornfield's paper, followed by this
statement: "The recent trend toward ... the old alphabetic or phonic
methods is viewed with alarm by educators...."
The most conspicuous example of this deadly warfare between the
entrenched “experts” and the advocates of common sense in reading is the
reception of the primer Reading With
Phonics by Hay and Wingo, published by the J. B. Lippincott Company. By
some miracle, this textbook company decided to jump into the fray and publish
the Hay-Wingo book, the only primer on the market today that is based firmly on
the alphabetic-phonetic principle. Well, the book was duly reviewed in Elementary English magazine by Dr. Celia
B. Stendler of the University of Illinois. I quote: “Reading With Phonics does not fit the modern conception of the
place of phonics in a reading program.... One wonders at the naiveté of the
authors.... One wonders, too, whether the authors have ever had the thrill of
seeing a group of children learn to read by the use of modem methods. The zest
with which these children approach reading and the zeal with which they read
will aknost certainly be lost if we turn the dock back twenty years with Reading With Phonies.” (This from someone who is all for
turning the clock back 3,5oo years!)
I’ll have more
to say later in this book about the Hay-Wingo primer which produces first-graders
reading news items from the daily paper — and about the zest and zeal with
which our children read:
Jack
ran out to see the truck.
It was red and it was
big—
very, very big.
It had come to take
Jack
far away to his new
home—
far away to his new
home
on a big farm.
In doing research for this book, I ran into exactly the same kind of
hostility. I wrote a letter to the National Council of Teachers of English,
asking for information on the phonetic method of teaching reading. I got a brief reply,
referring me to Dr. Paul Witty of Northwestern University (one of the top
word-method people) and to a pamphlet "What About Phonics?" by Dr. Alvina
Treut Burrows of New York University, which turned out to be violently
anti-phonies. I also wrote the U.S. Office of Education. That time I got a
somewhat longer reply, referring me to Dr. Edward W. Dolch of the University of
Illinois (another well-known word-method man) and to the same biased pamphlet
by Dr. Burrows.
At a later stage in my research I found an excellent paper by a Dr.
Agnew who had compared the results of teaching reading in the schools of Durham
and Raleigh, North Caroline. The monograph was published in 1939, at which time
the schools in Durham produced splendid results by teaching phonics. So I wrote
to the Superintendent of Schools in Durham, asking for information. The answer
was that the teaching of phonics there had been discontinued seven years ego.
Then I ran across a book by the Italian educator Dr. Maria Montessori,
published way back in 1912. Dr. Montessori,
who was a world-famous progressive kindergarten teacher, taught her little
Italian four-year-olds (!) the shapes and sounds of the letters of the alphabet
and had them reading within weeks. I found that there was a Child Education
Foundation in New York City carrying on Dr. Montessori work. I wrote to them,
asking about their method of teaching reading. The answer came back: “For a
number of years we have found other methods to be more effective, so have not
used Montessori.”
Now that I have gone through dozens and dozens of books on reading, I
know how well it all fits together. The primers and readers are keyed to the textbooks
on how to teach reading, and the textbooks are all carefully written so that every
teacher in the land is shielded from any information about how to teach
children anything about letters and sounds.
It's a foolproof system all right. Every grade-school teacher in the
country has to go to a teachers' college or school of education; every
teachers' college gives at least one course on how to teach reading; every
course on how to teach reading is based on a textbook; every one of those
textbooks is written by one of the high priests of the word method. In the old
days it was impossible to keep a good teacher from following her own common
sense and practical knowledge; today the phonetic system of teaching reading is
kept out of our schools as effectively as if we had a dictatorship with an
all-powerful Ministry of Education.
And how do you convince thousands of intelligent young women that black
is white and that reading has nothing to do with letters and sounds? Simple. Like
this:
First, you announce loudly and with full conviction that our method of
writing English is not based on
pronunciation. Impossible, you say? Everybody knows that all alphabetic systems
are phonetic? Oh no. I quote from page 297 of Reading and the Educative Process by Dr. Paul Witty of Northwestern
University: “English is essentially an unphonetic language.”
This is so ridiculous that it should be possible to just laugh about it and
forget it. But the reading “experts” have created so much confusion that it's necessary
to refute this nonsense. Well then: All alphabetic
systems are phonetic; the two words mean the same thing. The only trouble is
that English is a little more irregular than other language. How much more has
been established by three or four independent researchers. They all came up
with the same figure. About 13 per cent of all English words are partly
irregular in their spelling. The other 87 per cent follow fixed rides. Even the
13 per cent are not “unphonetic,” as Dr. Witty calls it, but usually contain
just one irregularly spelled vowel: done
is pronounced “dun,” one is pronounced “wun,” are is pronounced “ar,” and so on.
So our English system of writing is of
course phonetic, but has a few more exceptions to the rules than other
languages.
The next step in this great structure of nonsense and confusion is
careful avoidance of the teaching of the letters:
“Current practice in the teaching of reading does not require a
knowledge of the letters,” says Dr. Donald D. Durrell of Boston University. “In
remedial work, such knowledge is helpful.”
“The skillful teacher will be reluctant to use any phonetic method with
all children,” says Dr. Witty.
“The child
should be allowed to `typewrite' only after he has a certain degree of ability
in reading,” says Dr. Guy L. Bond of the University of Minnesota “Otherwise he
is apt to become too conscious of the letter-by-letter elements of words.”
And Dr. Roma
Garas of Teachers College, Columbia University, tells us simply and starkly: “In
recent years phonetic analysis of words at any level of the reading program
fell into disrepute.”
If they had their way, our teachers would never tell the children that there are letters and that each letter
represents a sound. However, that isn't quite possible for the simple reason
that a good many children are bright enough to find this out for themselves.
So, if systematic phonetics or phonics from the outset is taboo, there has to
be some sort of an answer when a child in second or third grade begins to
notice that the first letter in cat is
different from the first letter in sat. This
is called “phonetic analysis” and — lo and behold, — it does get mentioned in
the textbooks. For instance, if you turn to the index in Learning to Read: a Handbook for Teachers by Carter and McGinnis of
the Psycho-Educational Clinic of the Western Michigan College of Education, you
will find one lonely page reference
to “phonetic analysis.” Turning back to that page, you will learn that phonetic
analysis “grows out of the fact that words are made up of letters or letter combinations
that have known sounds. Phonetic analysis, then, is the process of associating
the appropriate sounds with the printed forms. At this stage of development
[third and fourth grade] emphasis should be placed upon beginning consonant
sounds."
Otherwise,
phonics is usually discussed in this literature as something that stupid and
ignorant parents are apt to bring up. Yes, I am rot joking: Our teachers are
carefully coached in what to answer parents who complain about the abandonment
of phonics.
For instance, let me quote from an “official” pamphlet on Teaching Reading by Dr. Arthur L Gates
(of Teachers College, Columbia University) published by the National Education
Association. “When a mother storms to the school,” writes Dr. Gates, “to
protest delaying the starting of the child to read or what she imagines is the
failure to teach good old phonics, it is likely that things have already
happened in the home which are having a disadvantageous —indeed, sometimes a
disastrous — influence on the pupil's efforts to learn. Had the mother
understood the school’s policy, provided it is a good one, the home life might
have been organized in such a way as to assist the pupil greatly." In other
words, if a parent complains that you don't teach her child the sounds of the
letters, tell her the child can't read because she has made his home life
unhappy.
That's what you get on the subject of phonetics in our literature on
the teaching of reading. And what do the books contain instead? With what do
they fill all those fat volumes with hundreds of pages if they don't mention
the letters and sounds of the alphabet? Very simple: Those books are not about
reading at all but about word guessing.
Because, you see, if a child isn't taught the sounds of the letters,
then he has absolutely nothing to go by when he tries to read a word. All he
can do is guess.
Suppose a child tries to read the sentence “I saw a kangaroo.” Suppose
he has never seen the word kangaroo before.
If he has been trained in phonics, he simply “sounds out” the k, the a, the ng, the a, the r, and the oo, and reads
“kangaroo” as easy as pie. (“Ah, kangaroo!” he says. Of course he has known the
meaning of the word for years.) But if he has no training in phonics, if the
meaning of the letters has been carefully hidden from him, he can only guess.
How can he guess? Well, the educators say, he can guess from context. With the
sentence “I saw a kangaroo” that is extremely difficult, however, because it
could just as easily mean “I saw a giraffe” or “I saw a flea” or “I saw a
piano.” So, the next best thing, the child looks at the top of the page to see whether there is a picture.
Usually in those factory-produced readers, when an animal is mentioned there is
a picture of it somewhere on the page, so ten to one he’ll find that the word
means “kangaroo.” And what if there isn't any picture? Well, then he has to
rely on the sound of the first letter k if
he knows that — or the length of the word — or its general
shape — or just sheer luck. He might guess “kangaroo” or he might guess
"plomber" or he might guess "forget-me-rot" or — most likely
— he might just sit there with a vacant look, waiting for the teacher to tell
him what the word is. He knows very well she’ll tell him eventually. Learning
to read, he knows, is guessing or waiting until you are told what the word
means.
You think I
exaggerate? On the contrary: I am describing exactly what I saw in one
classroom after another and what is detailed endlessly in all the textbooks on how
to teach reading. Lister to them:
“Little is
gained by teaching the child his sounds and letters as a first step to reading.
More rapid results are generally obtained by the direct method of simply
showing the word to the child and telling him what it is." (Irving H.
Anderson and Walter F. Dearborn, The
Psychology of Teaching Reading. Anderson is at the University of Michigan,
Dearborn is a professor emeritus of Harvard.)
“The simplest
solution when a child does not know a word is to tell him what it says." (Teaching
Primary Reading by Professor Edward A. Dolch, University of Illinois. The
triumphant italics are by Dr. Dolch)
«If the word is daddy, the pupil
may give the word father, or papa, or man, since the basal meaning is the same. If the word is the noun drink, the pupil may say water or milk or some other fluid. Similarly, words related to a common
situation or to a general topic, such as cow,
horse, pig, sheep, chicken, are likely to be mistaken for each other.
"Errors of this type are frequently regarded as evidence of carelessness
on the part of the pupil. In some instances he may be reprimanded for having
made a “wild guess,” when in fact, from the point of view of meaning the guess
is not at all wild. In the early stages of learning to read frequent errors of
this type are to be expected. They are ... evidence of keen use of the device
of guessing words from context." (Professor Arthur I. Gates, The Improvement of Reading, pp. 184-185.
This is generally considered the most authoritative text of them all.)
And finally, here is a perfect summary of the situation from Teaching the Child to Read by Bond and
Wagner. Professor Guv L. Bond is at the University of Minnesota.
The usual first unit of reading material is short and simple, rarely
running more than four or five pages and introducing but few words. It is
concerned with the common experiences of boys and girls of first-grade age
whose activities are to be followed throughout the first year. Usually the boy
and girl are introduced and some little story or incident told about them,
mainly through the pictures with but little reading material. The pictures in
the initial unit carry the story, and the words are so closely allied to the
picture story that they usually can be guessed by the children. The teacher's
major tasks during this time are to introduce the words in a meaningful fashion
so that the children have contextual clues to aid them in “guessing” the word
and to give repetition of the words so that those words may become the nucleus
of a sight vocabulary. The words should be recognized as whole words. It is detrimental
indeed to have the children spell or sound out the words at this stage.
Most of the modem readers have carefully worked out vocabulary controls
so that the child will not encounter many new words in comparison to the number
of words he actually reads. In various ways, which have been mentioned, the child
is prepared for reading those words. In fact, he has been either given the name
of the word or has been led to recognize the word
before he meets it in his purposeful reading activity. When, however, he does
have trouble with a word, that difficulty should not be focused upon as a
difficulty. The teacher should at this stage tell him the word or lead him to
guess it from the context.
What does all
this add up to? It means simply and clearly that according to our accepted
system of instruction, reading isn't taught at all. Books are put in front of
the children and they are told to guess at the words or wait until Teacher
tells them. But they are not taught
to read — if by reading you mean what the dictionary says it means, namely, “get
the meaning of writing or printing.”
Now you say that all this applies only to first grade. Not at all. If
you think that after this preparatory guessing game reading begins in earnest
in second grade, or in third, or in fourth, you are mistaken. Reading never starts. The guessing goes on and
on and on, through grade school, through high school, through college, through
life. It’s all they’ll ever
know. They'll never really learn to read.
When I started to work with Johnny, I didn't quite realize all this. In
my innocence, I gave him what I thought was an easy word for a twelve-year-old:
kid. He stared at it for quite some
time, then finally said “kind.” I tell you, it staggered me. Nobody born and
raised on the continent of Europe can easily grasp the fact that anyone can mistake kid for kind.
Later on, when
I had done a good deal of phonics work with Johnny, I gave him, as an exercise,
the word razzing. He hesitated, then
read it as realizing. I said, “Don't
guess, Johnny.” I don't know how many hundreds of times I must have said to
him, “Don't guess, Johnny.” To my mind, a remedial reading case is someone who
has formed the habit of guessing instead of reading.
You see,
remedial reading cases are harder to teach than first-graders for the simple
reason that they already have four or five or six years of guessing behind
them. It usually takes at least a year to cure them of the habit. There
wouldn't be any remedial reading
cases if we started teaching reading instead of guessing in first grade. (Did I
say this before? Forgive me. I have fallen into the habit of telling people the
simple facts about reading over and over again. It seems to be the only way.)
And how do the
educators explain ail the thousands and thousands of remedial reading cas.?
This is what really got me mad. To them, failure in reading is never caused by poor teaching. Lord no,
perish the thought. Reading failure is due to poor eyesight, or a nervous
stomach, or poor posture, or heredity, or a broken home, or undernourishment,
or a wicked stepmother, or an Oedipus complex, or sibling rivalry, or God knows
what. The teacher or the school are never at fault. As to the textbook or the
method taught to the teacher at her teachers' college — well, that idea has
never yet entered the mind of anyone in the world of education.
In the book How to Increase
Reading Ability by Professor Albert J. Harris of Queens College, New York
City, there are long descriptions of remedial reading cases with all sorts of
supposed causes and reasons — except the fact that Jimmie “confused m and n, u and v, b
and d, p and q, k and f, and y and w," and Bruce “was unfamiliar with all of the short vowel sounds and
with some consonant sounds." Fortunately Dr. Harris hit upon a phonics
book, the Hegge-Kirk Remedial Reading
Drills, and that was enough in
most cases to bring those unhappy children up to par in their reading. (The
Hegge-Kirk drills are what I finally used with Johnny. I’ll come back to that
book later on.)
There are also detailed case descriptions in The Improvement of Reading by Dr. Arthur L Gates, the widely used
text that I mentioned before. For instance, he tells about a ten-year-old girl
who “often confused the sounds of m
with n and had difficulty sounding
the letter y. She also confused l with i.” A seven-year-old boy, in a “test of ability to give sounds for
individual letters, did not know the following: f, d, z, r, m, l, q, u, w, h, n, and v.” An eight-year-old girl, “in a test
where she was asked to give the sounds for individual letters, missed the following: e, x, z, q, and g.”
And how do. Dr. Gates account for all this? He obliges us by giving each
of his cases a simple explanatory label. The first of the cases is labeled
Good Intellect, Poor Reading Techniques;
Sibling Rival, a Causal Factor.
The second case is headed
Reading Difficulties Resulting From
Parental Interference.
The third is a case of
Poor Reading Resulting Largely From
Parental Anxiety and Family Conflicts.
Dr. Gates, in
contrast to Dr. Harris, didn't give his remedial cases phonics and consequently
didn't help them; apparently he just gave the parents a good bawling out and
let it go at that.
Most educators, however, don't go quite as far as that. They do use
phonics in remedial cases — in dribs and drabs, testily, and rather furtively. Ordinary
children, they say, shouldn't be deprived of the privilege of guessing words;
but those poor unfortunate ones who didn't catch on to the guessing game —
well, let's teach them the sounds of the letters as a last resort, purely as an
emergency measure. (Remember the dictum by Dr. Durrell: “Current practice in
the teaching of reading does not require a knowledge of the letters. In
remedial work, such knowledge is helpful”) And so you find phonics discussed,
if at all, tucked away in a section dealing with remedial reading with a
careful explanation that this rather nasty medicine shouldn't be given to nice,
average children who can guess the few hundred words contained in the “basal
series.”
The irony is that phonics is also recognized when it comes to the
children above average — those that
somehow learn to read properly and effectively in spite of the way they were taught. Those boys and girls, the
reading experts tell us, have unusual phonic ability — which means that they
managed to figure out by themselves which letter stands for which sound. Of
course, you can’t really read at all if you don't know that; but for our
reading teachers it's a miraculous achievement, only to be explained by special
gifts and extraordinary graces.
Not long ago, in January, 1954, Dr. Ruth Strang of Teachers College,
Columbia University, published an article on the “Reading Development of Gifted
Children” in Elementary English. “It may
be,” she wrote, “that the phonetic approach is more appropriate for the
quick-learning than for the slow-learning and because of the former's greater
analytical ability.” (How she reconciled this observation with the fact that
phonic methods are the only thing that works with retarded children I don't
know.)
The article was based on statements by gifted boys and girls in junior
high school. Here are some of them:
“How did I learn to read? First my grandmother taught me, then I caught
on to certain words and got accustomed to sounding out words.”
“By very small words and sentences. Also by syllables and the letter's
sound.”
“In first grade the teacher was dismissed for teaching phonetics, but I
think phonetics has helped me very much in sounding out new words.”
It seems clear to me that those bright twelve- and thirteen-year-olds
know more about reading than all the faculties, students, and alumni of all of
our teachers' colleges and schools of education taken
together. And I don't think that
those children are a bit more gifted than your John. They were just luckier.
Just lucky enough to find out in time that learning to read means learning to
sound out words.
Aucun commentaire:
Enregistrer un commentaire
Aidez-moi à améliorer l'article par vos remarques, critiques, suggestions... Merci beaucoup.